Mistakes Were Made
September 19th, 2013

First Richard Friedman admits that the neurochemical imbalance is a myth. Now Nancy Andreasen tells The New Yorker that the biological revolution was oversold. Is that the Four Horsemen I see coming around the corner?

Andreasen isn’t just any psychiatrist. She has been one of the leading neuromantics of the past thirty years. She is the author of books like Brave New Brain, The Broken Brain, and The Creating Brain (detect a theme here?), and has made a career of breathless proclamations of psychiatry’s scientific triumph over the scourge of mental illness. In 1984 (!) she enthused that “The biological revolution in psychiatry has already occurred.” And in a way she was right. By then, psychiatrists and bureaucrats had already convinced each other that now that they had drugs that could treat serious mental illness, it would be a good idea to empty the psychiatric hospitals and scatter their patients to the streets of American cities to be treated as outpatients.

Four years later, fresh off that triumph, drug companies brought Prozac and then Paxil and Zoloft to market, and soon enough 20 percent of the American citizenry was taking psychiatric drugs to treat those biochemical imbalances that nearly all of us believed in. In the meantime, as Freudian theory was bashed by everyone from a retired Berkeley professor of  English to the author of the DSM-III to Peter Kramer, the idea that psychological symptoms had actual meaning, that the things people thought and felt and said were something more than the random noise generated by the broken brain, and that people who treat the mentally ill should actually listen to what they have to say–that idea all but disappeared in favor of listening to the drugs and to the brains they theoretically treated.

Viva la revolucion.

All along, of course, people (including me) have been counseling restraint, wondering if it was really a good idea to deprive the mentally ill of their refuges, or to incorporate psychoactive drugs into our daily diet, or to tell the people over and over again about chemical imbalances that were invented in marketing departments rather than discovered in the lab (especially when the measured results of the treatments were equivocal at best) or to abandon the notion that what we think and feel and say is meaningful, and in so doing to undermine a notion of human agency that in its half millennium had brought us democracy, science, technology, and the notion of universal human rights. For their note of caution, these people were routinely diagnosed: as ignorant dead-enders, as Popes against Galileo, as guild members fighting for market share, as counter-revolutionaries standing in the way of progress.

And now, in a New Yorker piece about how the content of delusions seems to be changing with the times (and how could it not? Schizophrenic people in ancient times didn’t have a CIA to worry about, so they heard God’s voice instead) and therefore may indeed be of interest, Andreasen says the biological revolution went too far. “You can’t treat a person as a purely biological creature,” she says. Which is pretty obvious to you and me, but when you’ve made your living saying (and doing) (and encouraging your peers to do) the opposite, it can be an epiphany.

Now, it would be nice if Andreasen took a page out of Al Frances’s book and acknowledged her role in the overselling of biological psychiatry, or at least showed some understanding of the vast disaster it has caused in some people’s lives, and,. in a diffuse way, in society at large. Where is the remorse for having let herself get so carried away? Where is the acknowledgment of the effects of this mistake?

On the other hand, Antonio Egas Moniz didn’t return the Nobel he won in 1939 for inventing the transorbital lobotomy.

Maybe Andreasen did make a thorough account of herslef this and the New Yorker chose not to report it. But I sort of doubt it. Because listen to her explanation of why it might be a good idea to treat a person as, well, a person.

When you have a therapeutic relationship, you can manage the dosage better. You can talk to the patient about their side effects. Compliance goes up. The patient gets better more quickly.

It’s important to have a “therapeutic relationship,” in other words, so you can get better at prescribing drugs. Listening to the patient is just another way of  listening to Prozac, which is just another way of listening to the brain, which, poor broken thing, is stuck trying to communicate with language. The rediscovery of the patient is only in service of making the revolution more efficient, or at least as efficient as it can be while we await the day we can cut out middleman completely.

And while I don’t want to bite the hand that feeds me occasionally too hard, I have to point out that the New Yorker, in turning to Andreasen and Ben Sadock and Joel Gold to enunciate the return of the repressed (not to mention in choosing such a fluffy example, the Truman Show syndrome) has sort of missed the boat here. Not that I have anything against them (and Sadock in particular has been very cordial in email with me), but they appear to be just discovering (or rediscovering) the idea that content has meaning and are hardly the most effective or knowledgeable advocates of that idea. Meanwhile, people like Gail Hornstein have been writing about the importance of the content of delusions for years–and not just to say that they draw from the well of popular culture or that they have value as therapeutic adjuncts, but that they can help us understand the delusional person and maybe even help him or her. Or Mary Olson and Jaakko Seikkula, who are old hands at taking the psychotic person as more than the unfortunate victim of a broken brain.  Listening to them–now, that would be a revolution.

 

 




That reminds me
September 17th, 2013

My little aside about obesity as a disease reminded me of a story that illustrates just about everything that’s wrong with everything.

Back in the early aughts there was great hope for drugs called CB-1 receptor antagonists to treat obesity, notably one invented by Sanolfi-Aventis and called rimonabant. The idea was simple, or at least it was self-evident to anyone who might have smoked marijuana. The CB-1 receptor is stimulated by pot. (CB stands for cannabinoid; we have many different cannabinoid receptors in the brain, which is how pot does its thing.) Pot causes munchies. Therefore, if you block activity at the receptor, you can cause anti-munchies. Loss of appetite leads to less eating leads to weight loss, less diabetes and other metabolic diseases, and no more overflow on airplane seats. A win-win.

Except for one thing. On exactly the same logic, blocking cannabinoid receptors is going to cause anti-highness, or what the doctors like to call depression. I mean, it’s pretty obvious that a neural system that, when stimulated, makes you feel excellent is going to make you feel sort of lousy if it is suppressed. What were those scientists smoking when they forgot this?

And sure enough, although rimonabant was effective at making rats lose weight, and even some people, it also caused depressed mood with enough frequency and severity that the clinical trial was halted and the drug removed from the pipeline.

And here’s the funny part. In the wake of rimonabant’s demise there was a series of papers like this one  offering solace to researchers by recounting what they had learned through the CB-1 antagonist adventure, and what other adventures lay in store. One of those suggested adventures was a foray into the world of depression treatment, once again on the same logic. As the scientists pointed out, “the reported clinical evidence that the CB1 antagonist rimonabant increases the risk of depression and anxiety, support the notion that the endocannabinoid system represents a novel target in the treatment of mood disorders.” Find a drug, in other words, that targets the cannabinoid receptors, and you might be onto something.  You might discover a compound that makes people feel better, gives them pleasure even. I wonder what they’ll call it. Here’s my vote: marijuana.

 

 




In Other News
September 16th, 2013

The State of New Jersey has become the second state to outlaw reparative therapy for homosexuality. That’s what its advocates call it, at least they used to. Now they generally dare not speak its name, instead calling it “sexual orientation change effort.” I like the name some gay activists have invented for it: “pray the gay away.”  Whatever you call it, it’s no longer legal in New Jersey, at least not when it’s practiced upon minors, and just ten days after the law passed, the Ninth Circuit Court of Appeals upheld a similar law passed in California a couple of years back. It was a bad end of August for the idiots out there who worry about who is sticking what into whom–a subject whose minute details they seem awfully intimate with and preoccupied by, especially when it concerns nubile youth.

In case it is not clear, I have no use for people who argue, from natural law or Biblical law or evolutionary doctrine, that homosexuality is a Bad Thing. Love is rare and life is hard and it is cruel to tell people who find themselves attracted to or in love with members of their own sex that they should stifle themselves. We’ll all be plenty stifled when we’re dead.

In the meantime, you could do a lot worse for a watchword than “only connect.” In fact, when I attended a conference run by the National Association for Research and Theapy of HOmosexuality a few years back in Orlando, I was pretty sure I was seeing a fair amount of connecting going on among the attendees, many of whom had supposedly been cured of their gayness–a fleeting glance in an elevator, hugs that went on just a tad long, the constant attention to the details of erotic life. Packed together into a windowless room across the road from the Magic Kingdom, their stifled desire seemed to be breaching its banks. I even got asked out on a date (or so I wanted to call it) by a tall dark man from Azerbaijan, an ex-gay, ex-Muslim, born again psychiatrist who wanted to go see Borat with me but at the last minute got a better offer. Too bad. I am sure we would have loved watching the wrestling scene together.

But I digress. Much as I don’t like these people, I think you have to admit they have a point in their opposition to these laws. Not on the mindless libertarian principle they cite–that these laws infringe on the therapist’s right to choose the proper treatment, the patient’s right to choose to change something about him or herself, and parent’s rights to look after their children as they see fit. I mean, all of that is true, the laws do this. That is their explicit point, and explicitly because  they hold that sexual orientation is not in the domain of things that can be chosen. Therefore, they say, reparative therapy is not the upholding of freedom, but rather a way to oppress people. Outlawing it is no worse than outlawing bloodletting or trepanation or, for that matter, slavery.

In real life, I am sure this is exactly how reparative therapy works. Parents scared to death that their children will grow up to be gay, and encouraged by their pastor or someone’s aunt, haul the kid they just discovered in an illicit embrace with one of their same-sex schoolmates to the nearest therapist who the pastor or aunt knows to have helped someone go straight. The point of the therapy is to change the child’s desire such that he wants what he is supposed to want, and what his parents (and probably at least sometimes the kid himself) wishes he wanted. It is inherently oppressive, in other words.

Now it turns out that sexual desire is pretty obdurate. You can shock and threaten and torture people. You can show them same-sex porn, tell them to masturbate to it, and then, right before they come, switch the  to the opposite-sex channel. You can pay sex surrogates to copulate with them in hopes that a little professional swiving will show them the light. None of that seems to work very well. People want what they want.

But that’s not to say that sexual orientation never changes. It’s not even to say that sexual orientation exists, at least not in the same way that lefthandedness or eye color exist. The idea that we have to be one way or the other is a relatively recent one, maybe 150 years old, and it’s all caught up in Victorian Europe and its ideas about taking the measure of the species and cultivating it like some kind of fine garden. And there are notable exceptions, especially among women, at least some of whom seem to be relatively fluid in their sexuality. The problem here is most likely in the notion of a fixed orientation, which fits many different political agendas, but does not seem to fit us very well. Unfortunately, the notion of a fixed orientation is also the key to many of the advances gay people have made in the last half century, starting with the deletion of homosexuality from the DSM. If gay people were indeed born that way, then it is foolish as well as inhumane to try to change them.

So here we have another one of those myths, or noble lies, or whatever you want to call them–this time, that sexual orientation can’t change. If this is what it takes for your average state legislator to pass laws ending discrimination or allowing gay marriage or even outlawing conversion therapy, then I say let’s not worry too much that it is bullshit. But just as “born that way” can become a burden when events prove it incorrect, so too outlawing therapy based on the notion that people can’t change might also prove to be a bad move. After all, it seems like every day neuroscientists are telling us about some other capacity or tendency that is beyond our control. Just today, in fact, NPR reported on the American Medical Association’s decision to make obesity an official disease. “We’re all wired in slightly different ways,” a doctor told the reporter. Which means, says another doc, “if you have that genetic susceptibility to gain weight, you will gain weight easily, no matter what.” Fat people were born that way, in other words.

But that doesn’t mean they can’t be cured. As the NPR story makes clear, the disease designation will make it easier for patients to get treatment; that. as I’ve said millions of times, is exactly what it means to call something a disease. It will also make gastric reduction surgery and/or anti-obesity drugs more attractive to more people. “Born that way” is increasingly not so much a justification for not changing something as it is a way to put that change into the hands of medical science, whose unacknowledged ability to reach deep into our persons, into our sense of agency, into our ways of experiencing the world, also seems to be increasing. It’s an invitation to the geneticists and the brain docs and their drug company patrons to figure out how to intervene, and I guarantee you that somewhere some scientist is slaving away at the question of how to change sexual orientation. It’s as if the category itself had been invented so that it could be eradicated.

I suppose if this happens, at least some of us will be glad for those laws, although once a “cure” for homosexuality is discovered, the laws will be seen as outdated and scientifically unsound. In the meantime, I for one am glad that the born that way myth reigns. This is an extremely stupid society living through what is perhaps one of its stupidest eras, and I’ll take any glimmer of light. Surely the idea that people should be free to love whom they love is way too radical for us. But mark my words. Live by the lancet, die by the lancet.

And in the meantime, if state legislators really want to protect children from oppression masquerading as mental health treatment , maybe they ought to turn their attention to the unfolding atrocity of children on antipsychotic drugs. Or maybe to the fact that 14 percent of American families are living in poverty, and 20 percent of American children. But that’s for a different day.




More from the mailbag
September 5th, 2013

Remember when the New Yorker didn’t run letters to the editor? I’m guessing it was because Mr Shawn thought it would be vulgar, but whatever the reason, the practice frustrated enough people that there were, as I recall, a number of other places where letters were published, arch little columns. There was probably one in Spy, but who knows? This was before the Internet, I’m afraid, so I can’t be more specific or provide links. And given my multifarious character flaws, I may just be making this up.

Well, times have changed. The magazine has a letters page, and the website…well, the website would make Mr Shawn blanch, what with its open invitation to the hoi polloi to express itself ungrammatically, unconcisely, and with numerals. And my post on the elements blog has elicited  quite a few comments (in addition to the surprising amount of email I’ve gotten, much of it vitriolic, much of that from psychiatrists, like the one reproduced here).

Now I suppose I could just reply on the New Yorker’s website, but that would be vulgar. It would convey that I am thin-skinned, that I care what the hoi polloi think, that I want everyone to love me and get my feelings hurt when they don’t, and I just don’t want my editors there to get the wrong idea. But I am all of those things, of course, so I must respond to at least a couple–as in the old days, in a different venue.

First, there are a number of comments about placebos and antidepressants, of which the best is surely this one

The canard that SSRI’s are equivalent to placebo, at least in moderate to severe depression, has been repeatedly and convincingly rebutted (1,2). Paul Krugman calls these sorts of arguments “Zombie lies” –

(1,2) are references to a couple of papers that reanalyzed the FDA data used by Irv Kirsch to show that half of the clinical trials run by drug companies for antidepressants failed to show a therapeutic effect of the drug, and in the successful ones the drug effect was just barely significant, statistically speaking. These papers argued that in moderate to severe depression, the drugs were effective, and that the problem was really that too many mildly depressed people were getting into the clinical trials.

Whatever the virtues of this argument (and it may have some, but it is hardly a conclusive rebuttal, let alone a killer argument), what’s important here is that while some people do conclude from the FDA data that SSRIs are “equivalent to placebo,” I’m not one of them, and in the New YOrker piece all I write is that they have “proven to be no more effective than placebos in clinical trials.” That is not a canard, let alone a zombie lie. It’s just what the FDA data show, no more, no less. I actually believe, as I have written elsewhere, that the SSRIs are powerful psychotropic drugs whose effects don’t show up well on trials with depressed people because their main effects aren’t on mood (which is the industry’s fault; they want the drugs to be antidepressants because that’s a good way to market them).

So why are defenders of the psychopharmacological regime so eager to say that I’m attributing an equivalency? Because they cling to those reanalyses like Titanic passengers to the lifeboats, and unless they can make it sound like I’m saying what I am not saying and don’t believe, and what is just plain stupid to say, then they are just left to flail about in the frigid waters.

Here’s another.

Nice article, but it’s explicitly false that there’s “nothing new on the horizon.” Brain network analysis is finally putting a meaningful (although still incomplete) neurobiological face on mental disorders. For example, with depression, remarkable results are being achieved with ketamine (e.g., http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0044799), a broad glutamate antagonist that reduces cortical over-activity. Functional connectivity imaging has now strongly implicated over-active cortical control, rumination, and decision-making networks in depression (http://www.pnas.org/content/107/24/11020.short), and this can be addressed chemically or through other more direct means. Similar stories are emerging w/ other mental disorders.

I like this formulation: “meaningful neurobiological face on mental disorders.” It’s just spooky enough, no doubt inadvertently so, to function as its own critique of the neurobiological approach to mental suffering. Myself, I think the human face is meaningful enough, would much rather gaze into someone’s eyes than into their gray matter. But it’s the ketamine thing I want to respond to. Speaking of lifeboats, this ketamine research has been the great white matter hope for neurobiologists of depression. The research shows that a single dose of ketamine relieves depression. (THis, by the way, is another accidental finding–anesthesiologists have observed that depressed patients on whom they have used ketamine often emerge from the surgery feeling better.)

But two things: first, just because ketamine is active at glutamate receptors and modulates “cortical over-activity,” that, to paraphrase tje New York Times’s Richard Friedman, does not mean that glutamate or cortical overactivity are the cause of depression. Nor can a target as amorphous as cortical overactivity, whatever that is (and how do we know it’s “over-activity”? how much cortical activity is normal? will cortical activity become the new chemical imbalance?), be particularly useful to drug makers–I’m sure that Thorazine reduces cortical activity too. So it’s only the Prozac problem, shifted from serotonin to ketamine. The second thing is that no one in the ketamine world seems to want to talk about the psychological effects of ketamine, which has been known for decades as a psychedelic drug. Euphoria and other effects are sometimes noted as adverse reactions in the studies, but the scientists are so enamored of the notion that biochemistry must be the signal and psychology merely the noise that they ignore the experience of the patient completely. There’s a whole world of meaning in that elision; mayb there’s more going on here than brain chemistry. (I’d also point out that the other drug that seems to relieve depression in a single dose is psilocybin, and at least in those studies they don’t sidestep the question of the actual effects on the subject’s psyche.)

And the third thing: when this commenter writes, “this can be addressed chemically or through other more direct means,” am I the only one who gets a little worried?
Finally, this

Psychiatry is hardly unique when it comes to serendipitous discoveries.  The same can be said of virtually every other therapeutic area.  What makes the “chemical imbalance” theory compelling is not its scientific foundation but its rhetorical appeal.  The same can said of the LDL-HDL (bad cholesterol, good cholesterol) theory.  What Greenberg and other sophisticated skeptics often lose sight of, however, is that for many — myself included — these drugs have made an extraordinary difference.  For patients like me, the question is not so much “how these drugs may work” but rather “do they work for me?”

 

It is, of course, true that every medical field has its accidental discoveries. Only in psychiatry, however, have none of those accidents eventually been explained. More significant is the point about how the drugs work. It is of course not fair to say that I have lost sight of this fact, although it is true that I don’t directly address it in the New Yorker piece. (And interesting that in the absence of an explicit statement to the contrary, the assumption is that I think psychiatric drugs don’t make a difference.) But what the commenter points out is one of the deeper scandals of the psychopharmacological era. The researchers have been so busy trying to fit the empirical evidence into the magic bullet model that they have failed to give us a solid account of what the drugs do for people. Of course, and apologizing for two Titanic references in one post, that would steer them dangerously close to  the recreational drug iceberg, for they would quickly find themselves talking with people about how the drugs changed their sense of who they are.




Where the bear shits in the buckwheat
September 4th, 2013

That’s one of my friend Russell Perry’s expressions. Russ is an old school mechanic, the kind who tries to actually fix cars rather than just swapping out expensive parts until the car runs. He’s got forearms like Popeye’s, and even though he’s 74 years old, he’s still at it full time. But he does spend part of each day telling stories to whoever happens to drop by, and sometimes those stories include a person telling another person something that should be obvious but nonetheless needs to be pointed out, like the fact that it’s not okay to drive with the oil light on. Which is what the expression is meant to convey, since, if you think about it (and I have), bear shit in buckwheat is not very hard to discern.

Anyway, a reader of a piece posted last night on the New Yorker’s elements blog has located the ursine excrement in the grain for me in an email. I’ll reproduce it in its entirety, save for his name, which I will change to Dr. Benway.  I will tell you that he’s a psychiatrist, although, like the bear shit, that is probably obvious.

 

Message: Your “damn book” tweet drew my attention. [I tweeted this morning about a mistake in the article, and about how I should have known better since the correct version was in my “own damn book.”] I read “The Psychiatric Drug Crisis”. Although you make a few valid points, you are obviously a polemicist and a gadfly, not a scientist. Until we are permitted to do biopsies on living brains (which I of course oppose), or come up with the equivalent, psychiatry will remain an art as well as a science.

Many discoveries in medicine (e.g. penicillin) were also made via serendipity. Anybody in my profession, which has plenty of flaws like any other, knows that true depression, on average, will remit in about 6 months. Antidepressants shorten that, as long as a proper diagnosis is made in the first place and a competent practitioner, by a necessary trial and error process, finds a medication that is at least reasonably effective and has a tolerable side-effect profile.

Rather than flacking your latest book on Twitter and elsewhere, I suggest you learn some science. You are obviously a gifted journalist, and any target is legitimate, but the article referred to above is deceptive, irresponsible, and self-serving. Any competent psychiatrist who is not wearing blinders knows that simplistic explanations of mental illness that are tied to neurotransmitters have been proven wrong. So what? That’s how science proceeds. We all know about the mind-brain debate. And incidentally, some of the most vocal and influential critics of prevailing models of mental illness and the practice of psychiatry have been psychiatrists themselves, like Thomas Szasz, R.D. Laing, and E. Fuller Torrey. At least they didn’t cobble their writings together based upon Cliff’s Notes or Psychiatry for Idiots.

Further, we know that mental illnesses can be misdiagnosed, overdiagnosed and even mislabeled. Again, that’s the way the world works. Physicians and scientists are human and the entire medical profession is currently under attack by persons much better qualified than you.

As to your comments about people being told that their condition is due to a chemical imbalance, would you rather they be told that they are crazy? Most of these “explanations”, even if ultimately proven wrong, are made in good faith, based upon what is hypothesized or “known” at the time.

Irresponsible, inaccurate (imipramine=Elavil)journalism like yours, as exemplified by the article in question, does a disservice. Just because a “chemical imbalance” may be an inadequate, or even false theory of mental illness, doesn’t mean that progress has not been made, and that, again, antidepressants work very well when judiciously utilized, but don’t work for everyone. That’s why treatment-resistant depression is a recognized entity.

In my opinion, psychiatrists have been their own worst PR people, and should be more proactive in informing the public about new developments, etc. Freudian theory and the dogmatism of its dwindling adherents have done tremendous damage to the public perception of psychiatry and a significant percentage of the population don’t know the difference between a psychiatrist, a psychologist, an analyst, a psychiatric social worker, a counselor and a therapist.

You would be doing the world a much greater service by presenting unbiased and constructive information instead of the poorly-targeted, deceptive, superficial and semi-informed work you bring out. BTW, I am aware of your personal experience with treatment. That experience is in and of itself a potential double-edged sword with respect to the objectivity of your views regarding psychopharmacology.

You have lost all credibility with me, and I certainly won’t be buying your “damn book”.

I really like this letter. It’s heartfelt, direct, and nicely written. He’s correct in many ways–surely that I am trying to sell books on Twitter and elsewhere, that I am a gadfly (although I’m not sure I am a polemicist) and not a scientist, that my article confused the names of two antidepressants, and that he probably isn’t going to be buying my damn book–although that’s a pity, because he might learn a thing or two from it. Plus he’s honest enough to admit how closed-minded he is.

But he does have a few things wrong. I never said progress hadn’t been made. I didn’t argue that psychiatry was the only medical specialty indebted to accident, and I didn’t cobble together my argument from Cliff;s Notes or Psychiatry for Idiots. I cobbled them together from two books that collectively took me more than five years to write, and which each run more than 400 pages with extensive bibiliographies. But that doesn’t matter, except for one thing: that accusation is, like the rest of the email, a nearly perfect example of one of the most common species of reactions to my work about psychiatry. Let’s call it the ad hominem gambit, in which the shortcomings of my character are somehow taken to invalidate what I am saying, and which reaches an apotheosis in this letter. I don’t think all these adjectives–self-serving, irresponsible, deceptive, superficial, polemical, ill-informed, dangerous, and personally experienced with treatment–have ever been gathered together in a critique of my stuff before. So my hat’s off to Dr.  Benway for his efficiency.

But go ahead and read my article and then try to find exactly what  Dr. Benway is saying is incorrect, other than that stupid error (now corrected) about the antidepressant names.  I think what my article says is fairly simple: the drug industry is giving up on making psychiatric drugs because mental disorders simply don’t respond to medication in the same way that many (but not all) physical disorders do. Or, to put it another way, the germ theory of disease, out of which the drug industry grew and which it adopted as its model, doesn’t work very well when it comes to psychological suffering. While once it was thought that neurotransmitter imbalances would play the same role as germs in understanding and treating mental illness, this has proven to be far too simplistic an approach, and the drug companies, having watched the wagon to which they hitched their hopes crash and burn, are unhitching themselves as fast as they can. And, I speculate, it’s possible that consumers, who have yet to fully understand that the neurotransmitter imbalance theory (NIT) works better as myth than as science, may not be far behind.

This last bit is, I think, where Dr Benway’s ire comes from. I think people tend to think of “myth” as a derogatory term; surely he does. But even if a myth is not the truth, it isn’t a lie. It’s a powerful way of knowing the world. The NIT has served its purpose. As he himself points out, it’s better than just calling people crazy. But does that mean it’s the best account of mental suffering? More to the point, when your myth relies for its power on a claim to scientific accuracy, and when science proves the myth inaccurate, is it a good idea to cling to the myth? And even more to the point, when the psychiatrists have abandoned the myth, is it a good idea to continue to pr0mulgate it among the laity?

That’s the point at which Dr Benway and I disagree about good and bad faith. Of course the myth originated in good faith. Contrary to Dr Benway’s belief about me, I havce studied those origins deeply. I’ve talked extensively with the scientists, at least the ones still alive, who originated it, and found them to be brilliant, engaging, compassionate people who really thought that the NIT was going to be the key to the mysteries of mental illness. (And many of these discoveries and theories were indebted to LSD, which really predisposes me to be sympathetic.)

But once the myth been discredited, once doctors are saying one thing to each other and another thing to the rest of us, then I think that is a definition of bad faith. And when simply saying it out loud, as I did in the New Yorker blog, inspires this kind of vituperation, I think what we are seeing is the attempt to divert attention from the fact that there is nothing with which to replace the myth. This is indeed disconcerting and worrisome to doctors and patients alike. It calls psychiatry’;s authority into question and leaves patients to wonder what will become of them. (Only corporations are well-equipped to respond to the end of the myth, which they are doing by abandoning it.)  And it leaves psychiatrists without a response other than the one their training prepares them for: diagnosing the messenger. But regardless of my character, the bear shit is definitely in the buckwheat, and it really does stink.